Subscribe to New Scientist
Feeds

Home | In-Depth Articles

Out of your head: Leaving the body behind

Continue reading page |1 |2

THE young man woke feeling dizzy. He got up and turned around, only to see himself still lying in bed. He shouted at his sleeping body, shook it, and jumped on it. The next thing he knew he was lying down again, but now seeing himself standing by the bed and shaking his sleeping body. Stricken with fear, he jumped out of the window. His room was on the third floor. He was found later, badly injured.

What this 21-year-old had just experienced was an out-of-body experience, one of the most peculiar states of consciousness. It was probably triggered by his epilepsy (Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, vol 57, p 838). "He didn't want to commit suicide," says Peter Brugger, the young man's neuropsychologist at University Hospital Zurich in Switzerland. "He jumped to find a match between body and self. He must have been having a seizure."

In the 15 years since that dramatic incident, Brugger and others have come a long way towards understanding out-of-body experiences. They have narrowed down the cause to malfunctions in a specific brain area and are now working out how these lead to the almost supernatural experience of leaving your own body and observing it from afar. They are also using out-of-body experiences to tackle a long-standing problem: how we create and maintain a sense of self.

Dramatised to great effect by such authors as Dostoevsky, Wilde, de Maupassant and Poe - some of whom wrote from first-hand knowledge - out-of-body experiences are usually associated with epilepsy, migraines, strokes, brain tumours, drug use and even near-death experiences. It is clear, though, that people with no obvious neurological disorders can have an out-of-body experience. By some estimates, about 5 per cent of healthy people have one at some point in their lives.

People without any obvious neurological disorder can have an out-of-body experience

So what exactly is an out-of-body experience? A definition has recently emerged that involves a set of increasingly bizarre perceptions. The least severe of these is a doppelgänger experience: you sense the presence of or see a person you know to be yourself, though you remain rooted in your own body. This often progresses to stage 2, where your sense of self moves back and forth between your real body and your doppelgänger. This was what Brugger's young patient experienced. Finally, your self leaves your body altogether and observes it from outside, often an elevated position such as the ceiling. "This split is the most striking feature of an out-of-body experience," says Olaf Blanke, a neurologist at the Swiss Federal Institute of Technology in Lausanne.

Surprisingly pleasant

Some out-of-body experiences involve just one of these stages; some all three, in progression. Bizarrely, many people who have one report it as a pleasant experience. So what could be going on in the brain to create such a seemingly impossible sensation?

The first substantial clues came in 2002, when Blanke's team stumbled across a way to induce a full-blown out-of-body experience. They were performing exploratory brain surgery on a 43-year-old woman with severe epilepsy to determine which part of her brain to remove in order to cure her. When they stimulated a region near the back of the brain called the temporoparietal junction (TPJ), the woman reported that she was floating above her own body and looking down on herself.

This makes some kind of neurological sense. The TPJ processes visual and touch signals, balance and spatial information from the inner ear, and the proprioceptive sensations from joints, tendons and muscles that tell us where our body parts are in relation to one another. Its job is to meld these together to create a feeling of embodiment: a sense of where your body is, and where it ends and the rest of the world begins. Blanke and colleagues hypothesised that out-of-body experiences arise when, for whatever reason, the TPJ fails to do this properly (Nature, vol 419, p 269).

More evidence later emerged that a malfunctioning TPJ was at the heart of the out-of-body experience. In 2007, for example, Dirk De Ridder of University Hospital Antwerp in Belgium was trying to help a 63-year-old man with intractable tinnitus. In a last-ditch attempt to silence the ringing in his ears, Ridder's team implanted electrodes near the patient's TPJ. It did not cure his tinnitus, but it did lead to him experiencing something close to an out-of-body experience: he would feel his self shift about 50 centimetres behind and to the left of his body. The feeling would last more than 15 seconds, long enough to carry out PET scans of his brain. Sure enough, the team found that the TPJ was activated during the experiences.

Insights from neurological disorders or brain surgery can only take you so far, however, not least because cases are rare. Larger-scale studies are required, and to achieve this Blanke and others have used a technique called "own-body transformation tasks" to force the brain to do things that it seemingly does during an out-of-body experience. In these experiments, subjects are shown a sequence of brief glimpses of cartoon figures wearing a glove on one hand. Some of the figures face the subject, others have their back turned (see diagram). The task is to imagine yourself in the position of the cartoon figure in order to work out which hand the glove is on. To do this, you may have to mentally rotate you own body as one image succeeds another. As volunteers performed these tasks, the researchers mapped their brain activity with an EEG and found that the TPJ was activated when the volunteers imagined themselves in a position different from their actual orientation - an out-of-body position.

Continue reading page |1 |2
Issue 2729 of New Scientist magazine
  • Like what you've just read?
  • Don't miss out on the latest content from New Scientist.
  • Get 51 issues of New Scientist magazine plus unlimited access to the entire content of New Scientist online.
  • Subscribe now and save

If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.

Have your say

1960 Out Of Body Experience After Being Shot In Head

Thu Oct 08 04:00:35 BST 2009 by Steven Davis
http://www.citiesofpeace.com http://www.stopzion.com

At the age of ten in October, 1960 my mother had 3 straight nights of dreams about one of her sons (4, ages 8-15) had a head injury and she was comforted that he was going to be OK. The next day her husband accidently shot me in the head (left temple above ear). I was in a coma for 3 days and experienced many things with an Angel. I remember everything. I promised to only fear and listen to God and truth and not traditions of man. Everything witnessed in 1960 has come true (49 years and counting). When the docs removed the bullet they also removed part of my skull. Six months a metallic plate was placed over it. My experience was like that Saul (Apostle Paul) had.

1960 Out Of Body Experience After Being Shot In Head

Thu Oct 08 11:53:45 BST 2009 by Olaf

This is impossible. Once, when I recieved a ridiculous head trauma, the All-Father appeared to me and predicted I would have to reply to this NS message correcting you or else Sleipnir would trample me to death.

1960 Out Of Body Experience After Being Shot In Head

Thu Oct 08 13:49:40 BST 2009 by Eric Kvaalen

Typical reply of a modern man. (I assume this is not the Olaf Blanke the article.)

The article does not mention two things:

1. People who see things while out of body which they cannot see physically, and yet which are verified afterwards. (I know that often this does not work, but it's the cases where it does happen that pose the problem.)

2. Cases where the person was, to all appearances, dead. I gave the reference to an article on a case like this in The Lancet in a letter to NS a few years ago.

The work of the scientists mentioned in this article is interesting, but doesn't get to the real nub of the problem.

1960 Out Of Body Experience After Being Shot In Head

Sat Oct 10 08:10:16 BST 2009 by Eric Kvaalen

Typical reply of a modern man. (I assume you are not the Olaf Blanke of the article.)

The article does not mention two things:

1. People who see things while out of body which they cannot see physically, and yet which are verified afterwards. (I know that often this does not work, but it's the cases where it does happen that pose the problem.)

2. Cases where the person was, to all appearances, dead. I gave the reference to an article on a case like this in The Lancet in a letter which NS published few years ago.

The work of the scientists mentioned in this article is interesting, but doesn't get to the real nub of the problem.

All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.

If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.

Put yourself in his place

Put yourself in his place

Enlarge image

ADVERTISEMENT

Latest news

Adaptive games promise high scores for everyone

12:00 10 October 2009

A new breed of game aims to work out if a player is a fun junkie or a challenge-seeker, and adapt itself to please either

Review: Cracking the Einstein Code by Fulvio Melia

11:00 10 October 2009

The story of Roy Kerr, the man who solved Einstein's treacherous equations and paved the way for our understanding of black holes

Review: Botanica Magnifica by Jonathan Singer

10:00 10 October 2009

In this impressively sized and priced volume, Singer photographs orchids and other extraordinary plants as if they were celebrities on the red carpet

Today on New Scientist: 9 October 2009

18:00 09 October 2009

Today's stories on newscientist.com, at a glance, including: how to spot a liar with a sketch, why hypnosis reveals the secrets of our brains, and the world's smallest art prize

TWITTER

New Scientist is on Twitter

Get the latest from New Scientist: sign up to our Twitter feed

ADVERTISEMENT

Partners

We are partnered with Approved Index. Visit the site to get free quotes from website designers and a range of web, IT and marketing services in the UK.

Login for full access