SUBSCRIBE TO NEW SCIENTIST

Life

Feeds

Home |Life | In-Depth Articles

Timewarp: How your brain creates the fourth dimension

Continue reading page |1 |2 |3 |4

This implies that there is not a single "film roll" in the brain, but many separate streams, each recording a separate piece of information. What's more, this way of dealing with incoming information may not apply solely to motion perception. Other brain processes, such as object or sound recognition, might also be processed as discrete packets.

To investigate, VanRullen examined another neural function, called near-threshold luminance detection. He exposed his subjects to flashes of light barely bright enough to see, and found that the likelihood of them noticing the light depended on the phase of another wave in the front of the brain, which rises and falls about 7 times per second. It turned out that subjects were more likely to detect the flash when the wave was near its trough, and miss it when the wave was near its peak. The work was published in The Journal of Neuroscience earlier this year (vol 29, p 7869). "There's a succession of 'on' periods and 'off' periods of perception," VanRullen says. "Attention is collecting information through snapshots."

Perception is a sequence of 'on' and 'off' periods. We collect information through discrete snapshots

So it seems that each separate neural process that governs our perception might be recorded in its own stream of discrete frames. But how might all these streams fit together to give us a consistent picture of the world? Ernst Pöppel, a neuroscientist at the Ludwig Maximilian University in Munich, Germany, suggests all of the separate snapshots from the senses may feed into blocks of information in a higher processing stream. He calls these the "building blocks of consciousness" and reckons they underlie our perception of time (Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, vol 364, p 1887).

It's an appealing idea, since patching together a chronological order of events hitting our senses is no mean feat. Sounds tend to be processed faster than images, so without some sort of grouping system we might, say, hear a vase smashing before we see it happen. Pöppel's building blocks of consciousness would neatly solve this problem: if two events fall into the same building block, they are perceived as simultaneous; if they fall into consecutive buildings blocks, they seem successive. "Perception cannot be continuous because of [the limits of] neural processing," says Pöppel. "A space of 30 to 50 milliseconds is necessary to bring together in one time-window the distributed activity in the neural system."

Slices of time

There's some evidence to suggest this might be what happens. In one experiment, Pöppel analysed his volunteers' reaction times by measuring how quickly their eyes moved to follow a dot jumping across a computer screen. He found that their reactions seemed to follow a 30-millisecond cycle. If the dot moved any time within this cycle, it took until the end of the interval before the volunteers would react (Naturwissenschaften, vol 73, p 267). A similar cycle has since been observed when volunteers are asked to discern whether an auditory and a visual stimulus are simultaneous or consecutive - suggesting it may be at the root of Pöppel's building blocks of consciousness.

The brain's effort to maintain its timekeeping has implications for understanding some diseases. Schizophrenia, for example, could arise from an inability to coordinate information arriving from different parts of the brain (see "Delusions on demand"). A better knowledge of the way the brain integrates the discrete packets of information might provide further insights into this.

The question of discrete versus continuous perception is not the only challenge that time presents to neuroscientists. Many, including Eagleman, are concerned with the speed at which time seems to pass in different situations. Why do we feel that some, usually frightening, experiences last longer than others, even if objectively they occurred for the same number of seconds? Eagleman experienced this apparent slowing of time as an 8-year-old when he tumbled off a roof and broke his nose.

There are two possible explanations, he says. It could be a facet of the memory, or it could be that his brain's processing speed accelerated under the stress, making outside events appear to slow down in comparison. Decades later, he decided to replicate his experience under carefully controlled conditions.

After taking half a dozen members of his lab to a nearby amusement park and finding none of the rides scary enough, Eagleman found another outfit that offered a thrill ride known as a "suspended catch air device" which drops people from a 30-metre tower into a safety net below.

To measure the speed of his plucky volunteers' perceptions, Eagleman and his team designed a wrist-worn device they call a perceptual chronometer. An LED array on the face of the device displays a flickering single-digit number alternating with the negative of its image about 20 times per second. That would normally be too quick for a human to distinguish between the two images - you would just perceive all the elements of the LED array to be shining at once - but if their perceptual clocks of the terrified subjects accelerated even a little bit, Eagleman reasoned, the number would become visible.

Continue reading page |1 |2 |3 |4
Issue 2731 of New Scientist magazine
  • Like what you've just read?
  • Don't miss out on the latest content from New Scientist.
  • Get 51 issues of New Scientist magazine plus unlimited access to the entire content of New Scientist online.
  • Subscribe now and save

If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.

Have your say
Comments 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6

Time

Wed Oct 21 18:53:21 BST 2009 by Barry Soley

reading this article reminds me countless time I have looked at the clock and the second hand it not moving and then it starts, I am sure this takes longer than a second

Time

Wed Oct 21 20:54:52 BST 2009 by peter reynolds

Not a lot is said directly in the article about focus.

The essence of the human perception of time is focus. To identify and choose those external stimuli which are relevant to the situation pertaining at any particular time. To identify the rate of change of the most salient stimuli and organise the response of the body and brain to those stimuli. Different filters must be clicked in to exclude irrelevant stimuli.

Thus exciting events may be perceived more slowly because unnecessary detail is removed from the perception of the event itself. When the event occurrs more time is available to concentrate upon the salient feature of the event and so respond to it.

Thus the missing phrase in the above article is 'a priori'.

Our a priori perception allows us to focus upon particular events. - the brain effectively having an inbuilt lens for detail.

More time is thus available to focus upon relevant detail in any particular circumstance.

Also when recalling such memory our a priori conceptuality predisposes us to know that in the actual reality of the exciting event we existed in the same way in detail and temporaly as we do when we are subsequently recalling the event. However because during the event we had filtered out much of the normal apriori world that existed before and after the event we 'feel' that the actual timing of the event was slower than in the normal a priori world because during recall we would have to overlay the normal a priori rate of things so as to coordinate the memory with the current conditions of recall.

Thus also the thought of time as a 'feeling' is missing from the article.

This idea is central to Damasios theory of consciousness - that time exists in consciousness and thus is in essence a feeling.

Time

Wed Oct 21 21:06:23 BST 2009 by peter reynolds

NB there is a coherent physics without time - so it is not beyond the pale that one indeed considers time as an artefact of the 'brain'

Time

Fri Oct 23 15:10:15 BST 2009 by Rose

The essence of it seems to have been mentioned in the article - the neurons have to be "recharged" between firings, so what we may be experiencing is a slide show between rechargings, sight and sound showing some lack of sync because of the difference in input between speed of light and speed of sound (?), and apparent subjective experiences can be due to adrenalin influence. Schizophrenics might have a neurotransmitter problem which leaves the scrambling for integration.

Time

Sat Oct 24 01:22:33 BST 2009 by Dennis
http://freetubetv.net

I see someone is heavily influenced by Stephen Hawking's work on time. The idea of time occurring at a much faster or slower pace than human cognition can comprehend is fascinating but not sure how they can really prove that.

Time

Wed Oct 21 21:19:29 BST 2009 by David N. Hake

Thank God!!! I'm not the only one! The 'second-hand' phenomena is really weird. The harder you look, the longer it takes to start, it seems. Thanks, everyone. David.H.

Time

Thu Oct 22 09:09:58 BST 2009 by Matg

Please don't stare too hard, I beg you. Time might go backwards and the effect could be catastrophic!

Time

Thu Oct 22 13:13:50 BST 2009 by peter reynolds

I have a theory that when you remember events you make time go backwards to the event itself.

Thats why a memory is never clear - because quantum mechanics precludes you remebering the exact event but rather you can actually recall information about the event which was not present to your conscious self at the time of the event. This means that this time travel backwards does not alter the present. eg when you travel backward in time to the event - you actually really do see the event again but from a slightly different perspective.

This time travel is an ability of mind rather than brain, so is very difficult to measure precisely but its affects can be felt.

It may be possible to travel back in time and reinvestigate an event.

Perhaps this is done in hypnosis.

Time

Thu Oct 22 09:37:27 BST 2009 by James Fewings

I have done that so many times! Yes it does take longer than a second to start again or at least it seems like it.

Time

Thu Oct 22 14:52:37 BST 2009 by ben
http://www.citeulike.org/user/agcs/article/3090361

This is due to saccades.

http://www.citeulike.org/user/agcs/article/3090361

"When voluntary saccadic eye movements are made to a silently ticking clock, observers sometimes think that the second hand takes longer than normal to move to its next position. For a short period, the clock appears to have stopped (chronostasis). Here we show that the illusion occurs because the brain extends the percept of the saccadic target backwards in time to just before the onset of the saccade. This occurs every time we move the eyes but it is only perceived when an external time reference alerts us to the phenomenon."

Time

Fri Oct 23 08:23:39 BST 2009 by Frank

In more 'joe sixpack'-terms. After you move your eyes fast, they are unable to collect information for a fraction of a second. When the eyes comes back 'online' the brain collects motion-information for an equal fraction of a second, and extrapolates the information backwards to create what things should have looked like and fills this fabricated visual information into your memory. Since the needle (or digit) didn't move while the brain was collecting info for the extrapolation, it won't be able to predict that it moved in the past either.

Time

Fri Oct 23 13:09:39 BST 2009 by Darrell Clarke

That happens with everyone, and when you think about how time has slowed or stopped, it goes again. Which makes me think (well actually i know from other experiences) that time is only constant in our concious mind, when where in a trance, not thinking, time around us slows down because of the amount of information we are processing. The more info we process, the slower time goes, almost to compensate. As we dream on the other hand, a 10 minute dream will last for hours, therefore time speeds up.

Time

Wed Oct 21 19:09:10 BST 2009 by Stephen Penny

If you want excitement, you should try being bombarded by a wartime "Moaning Minnie". When one lands on the side of your slit trench, you hear the 'pop' of the detonator, followed by the bang of the main charge followed by the scream of the metal casing being rent apart, it all takes a very brief time, but you can remember each as a separate event. Have they tried the experiment with soldiers on active service??

Time

Wed Oct 21 21:17:07 BST 2009 by peter reynolds

Again the brain focusses upon a very small part of the 'a priori' world when an explosive device threatens. When this memory later is filled in with and coordinated with the current and prior conditions of the a priori world it seems as though the timespan of the dangerous event must have been longer. - because our a priori world later fills in - at leisure -data which was not available to us whilst we were focussing upon the bomb exploding.

Time

Thu Oct 22 15:01:49 BST 2009 by aaa

Wouldn't it be a good idea to try the perceptual chronometer on people who had listened to the click train?

Slow Motion Attack

Wed Oct 21 19:10:27 BST 2009 by mel zimmer
http://www.youtube.com/user/iskandhar

At an anti Viet Nam war protest in NYC our small group was attacked repeatedly. When we went to leave they grabbed a frail young kid, one holding him and another began to hit him.

I slung my camera over my back and jumped on the back of the guy pummeling the kid. When he turned to come after me I tried to melt into the crowd but couldn't. As he charged at me I experienced the slow motion phenomena and was able to knock him out with one punch.

I would say it wasn't memory but the speeding up of messenger cells

Slow Motion Attack

Wed Oct 21 21:21:44 BST 2009 by peter reynolds

Suppose that rather than thinking of memory in frames per second one should think of it in terms of information per second. This would give a measure of the intesity of focus. This might indeed give us a new view into time itself as a flow of information.

Slow Motion Attack

Thu Oct 22 22:14:36 BST 2009 by Kathleen

I truly believe you are on the right track with your comment that we look at time as a flow of information. It melds an understanding of time as a state of consciousness that is supported by brain structure and function, and it is measurable. The big job will be identifying all input sources and nature of their inputs.

Slow Motion Attack

Thu Oct 22 10:45:52 BST 2009 by Ynot

Sounds like an adrenalin rush,and again the article doesn't mention adrenalin.

The click train effect is the same as listening to certain types of music while driving a car.As the beat speeds up so do you and the car.Nothing really new there.

Comments 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6

All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.

If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.

Understanding the brain's timekeeping mechanism could help understand symptoms of schizophrenia (Image: <a href="http://www.debutart.com/artist/metropolis" target="ns">Metropolis @ Debut Art)</a>

Understanding the brain's timekeeping mechanism could help understand symptoms of schizophrenia (Image: Metropolis @ Debut Art)

2 more images

ADVERTISEMENT

Brain-shrinking algae send sea lions off course

13:26 23 October 2009

MRI scans of sea lion brains have revealed how neurotoxins from algal red tides make them behave bizarrely

How green is your pet?

10:47 23 October 2009

It's time to wise up to the ecological pawprint of your furry friend – it may be doing more environmental damage than an SUV

The year's best wildlife photos

17:55 22 October 2009

The winners of the 2009 Veolia Environnement Wildlife Photographer of the Year competition have been announced. The best photographs from the competition are on show at the Natural History Museum, London, from tomorrow. Here are our favourites.

What's the point of a fake 500-day Mars mission?

14:47 22 October 2009

Volunteers are being asked to play astronauts on a simulated Mars mission – but it's not the only way to learn how long-haul spacefarers will cope

Latest news

Seven questions that keep physicists up at night

23:58 23 October 2009

From the nature of matter to that of reality itself, physicists are pondering the big questions at a 10-day physics festival in Canada

Obama says US in global race to develop clean energyMovie Camera

22:26 23 October 2009

At a speech at MIT on Friday, the president said that "the nation that wins this competition will be the nation that leads the global economy"

Today on New Scientist: 23 October 2009

18:00 23 October 2009

Today's stories on newscientist.com, at a glance, including: your pet's eco pawprint, the first "skylight" on the moon, and how to see through walls

Augmented reality system lets you see through wallsMovie Camera

17:21 23 October 2009

If walls could be transparent, there would be fewer road accidents at blind corners. An augmented reality system could just make that come true

TWITTER

New Scientist is on Twitter

Get the latest from New Scientist: sign up to our Twitter feed

ADVERTISEMENT

Partners

We are partnered with Approved Index. Visit the site to get free quotes from website designers and a range of web, IT and marketing services in the UK.

Login for full access